Receptive aphasia

By: Audiopedia

Receptive aphasia, also known as Wernicke’s aphasia, fluent aphasia, or sensory aphasia, is a type of aphasia traditionally associated with neurological damage to Wernicke’s area in the brain. However, the key deficits of receptive aphasia do not come from damage to Wernicke's area; instead, most of the core difficulties are proposed to come from damage to the medial temporal lobe and underlying white matter. Damage in this area not only destroys local language regions but also cuts off most of the occipital, temporal, and parietal regions from the core language region. People with receptive aphasia are unable to understand language in its written or spoken form, and even though they can speak with normal grammar, syntax, rate, and intonation, they cannot express themselves meaningfully using language. Receptive aphasia is not to be confused with Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Presentation When we want to speak, we formulate what we are going to say in Wernicke’s area, which then transmits our plan of speech to Broca’s area, where the plan of speech is carried out. Wernicke’s Area is located posterior to the lateral sulcus, typically in the left hemisphere, between the visual, auditory, and somesthetic areas of the cerebral cortex. A person with this aphasia speaks normally but uses random or invented words; leaves out key words; substitutes words or verb tenses, pronouns, or prepositions; and utters sentences that do not make sense.

They have normal sentence length and intonation but without true meaning. They can also have a tendency to talk excessively. A person with this aphasia cannot understand the spoken words of others or read written words. Speech is preserved, but language content is incorrect. Substitutions of one word for another are common. Comprehension and repetition are poor.

Patients who recover from Wernicke’s aphasia report that, while aphasic, they found the speech of others to be unintelligible. And, despite being cognizant of the fact that they were speaking, they could neither stop themselves nor understand their own words. The ability to understand and repeat songs is usually unaffected, as these are processed by the opposite hemisphere. Patients also generally have no trouble purposefully reciting anything they have memorized. The ability to utter profanity is also left unaffected, however the patient typically has no control over it, and may not even understand their own profanity. Damage to the posterior portion of the left hemisphere’s superior and middle temporal lobe or gyrus and the temporoparietal cortex can produce a lesion to Wernicke’s area and may cause fluent aphasia, or Wernicke’s aphasia. If Wernicke’s area is damaged in the non-dominant hemisphere, the syndrome resulting will be sensory dysprosody — the inability to perceive the pitch, rhythm, and emotional tone of speech.

Patients who communicated using sign language before the onset of the aphasia experience analogous symptoms. Luria's theory Luria proposed that this type of aphasia has three characteristics. 1) A deficit in the categorization of sounds.

Receptive aphasia

In order to hear and understand what is said, one must be able to recognize the different sounds of spoken language. For example, hearing the difference between bad and bed is easy for native English speakers. The Dutch language however, makes a much greater difference in pronunciation between these vowels, and therefore the Dutch have difficulties hearing the difference between them in English pronunciation. This problem is exactly what patients with Wernicke’s aphasia have in their own language: they can't isolate significant sound characteristics and classify them into known meaningful systems. 2) A defect in speech. A patient with Wernicke's aphasia can and may speak a great deal, but he or she confuses sound characteristics, producing “word salad” in extreme cases: intelligible words that appear to be strung together randomly. 3) An impairment in writing. A person who cannot discern sounds cannot be expected to write.

See also Aphasia Expressive aphasia Schizophasia Conduction aphasia Logorrhoea Agraphia Paragrammatism Transcortical sensory aphasia References Further reading Klein, Stephen B., and Thorne. Biological Psychology. New York: Worth, 2007. Print. Saladin, Kenneth S. Anatomy & Physiology: the Unity of Form and Function. New York: McGraw-Hill Higher Education, 2010. Print.

External links Aphasia Center of California in Oakland, CA, U.S.

What Does Stroke Recovery Look Like?

[ serene music] Strokes are very common, ah, in Canada there's approximately 50,000 new strokes a year. We have ah, programs like Stroke Recovery Canada designed to help people…

Views: 1 656 By: Marchofdimescda
Krystal visits the INR in Los Angeles 720p

All right. So, I am one week post my first injection. I am amazed at how well I'm doing. I'm taking better strides, my heel steps better I'm able to open my hand up When…

Views: 1 655 By: Institute of Neurological Recovery
PACE participant videos, part 2, section 2: Goals & criteria of successful…

In this visit, Dr. Esser will go into greater detail about the medications his patient will need to take. He will discuss why the medications are needed, their safety, and the goals…

Views: 567 By: NHLBI
Expressive Aphasia - Sarah Scott - Teenage Stroke Survivor

Okay so what's your name? Um Scott, oh no Sarah Scott. That's right. And how old are you? I can't. Try? I can't Ooh. You're nineteen. Nineteen. And what happened…

Views: 732 053 By: SymphUK
Labels: stroke with aphasia

Today the topic is "labels". There can be "positive" or "negative" labels. Labels can be "helpful" or "hinder" progress. "Label" is a noun: --a word or phrase that describes or identifies…

Views: 108 By: Christine Huggins
Parkinson's Disease Taylor French guest Mike Paulus

Parkinson's Disease Taylor French - Guest Mike Paulus Starting to walk again... The difference with vegetable extract - Hello, my name is Mike Paulus. The purpose of this video…

Views: 277 By: Pwits